$\require{mediawiki-texvc}$

연합인증

연합인증 가입 기관의 연구자들은 소속기관의 인증정보(ID와 암호)를 이용해 다른 대학, 연구기관, 서비스 공급자의 다양한 온라인 자원과 연구 데이터를 이용할 수 있습니다.

이는 여행자가 자국에서 발행 받은 여권으로 세계 각국을 자유롭게 여행할 수 있는 것과 같습니다.

연합인증으로 이용이 가능한 서비스는 NTIS, DataON, Edison, Kafe, Webinar 등이 있습니다.

한번의 인증절차만으로 연합인증 가입 서비스에 추가 로그인 없이 이용이 가능합니다.

다만, 연합인증을 위해서는 최초 1회만 인증 절차가 필요합니다. (회원이 아닐 경우 회원 가입이 필요합니다.)

연합인증 절차는 다음과 같습니다.

최초이용시에는
ScienceON에 로그인 → 연합인증 서비스 접속 → 로그인 (본인 확인 또는 회원가입) → 서비스 이용

그 이후에는
ScienceON 로그인 → 연합인증 서비스 접속 → 서비스 이용

연합인증을 활용하시면 KISTI가 제공하는 다양한 서비스를 편리하게 이용하실 수 있습니다.

노화에 의해 저하된 시공간 주의배분능력이 정서지각 편향성에 미치는 영향
The Influences of Deteriorated Visuo-spatial Attention Allocation Ability Caused by Aging on Emotional Perception Bias 원문보기

감성과학 = Science of emotion & sensibility, v.23 no.4, 2020년, pp.3 - 20  

김상엽 (고려대학교 심리학과) ,  정재범 (고려대학교 심리학과) ,  남기춘 (고려대학교 심리학과)

초록
AI-Helper 아이콘AI-Helper

본 연구의 목적은 노화에 따라 나타나는 시공간 주의배분능력과 정서지각 편향성의 변화에 대해 탐구하는 것이다. 본 연구에서는 시공간 주의배분능력을 측정하기 위해 useful field of view (UFOV) 과제와 정서지각 편향성을 측정하기 위해 정서지각과제(emotional perception task)를 사용하였다. 본 연구에는 노인 집단의 23명과 청년 집단의 25명의 총 48명의 실험참가자가 참여하였다. 실험 결과, UFOV 과제에서는 노인 집단이 청년 집단에 비해 느린 반응속도와 낮은 정답률을 보여 노인 집단이 청년 집단보다 저하된 시공간 주의배분능력을 가지고 있다는 것을 나타냈다. 또한, 정서지각과제에서는 노인 집단이 청년 집단에 비해 긍정적 정서지각 편향성과 부정적 정서지각 편향성 모두 크게 나타났다. 상관분석 결과에서는 정서지각과제의 정답률에 대한 부정적 정서지각 편향성이 UFOV 과제의 시야각 30° 조건의 반응속도와 유의미한 정적 상관관계가 나타났다(r=.289). 또한, 정서지각과제의 정답률에 대한 긍정적 정서지각 편향성은 UFOV 과제의 모든 시야각 조건들의 정답률과 유의미한 정적 상관관계가 나타났으며(r=.305, r=.322, r=.299), 반응속도와 유의미한 부적 상관관계가 나타났다(r=-.345, r=-.295, r=-.308). 이러한 결과는 노화가 저하된 시공간 주의배분능력 그리고 긍정적 정서지각 편향성과 부정적 정서지각 편향성의 증가와 관련있다는 것을 의미하고, 노화에 따라 나타나는 긍정적 및 부정적 정서지각 편향성은 저하된 시공간 주의배분능력과 관련성이 있는 것으로 여겨진다.

Abstract AI-Helper 아이콘AI-Helper

The purpose of this study was to investigate the effect of aging on visuo-spatial attention allocation ability and emotional perception bias. We used the useful field of view (UFOV) task to measure the visuo-spatial attention allocation ability and the emotional perception task to measure positive a...

주제어

참고문헌 (96)

  1. Armony, J. L., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). Modulation of spatial attention by fear-conditioned stimuli: An event-related fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 40(7), 817-826. DOI: 10.1016/s0028-3932(01)00178-6 

  2. Awh, E., & Jonides, J. (2001). Overlapping mechanisms of attention and spatial working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(3), 119-126. DOI: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01593-x 

  3. Ball, K. K., Beard, B. L., Roenker, D. L., Miller, R. L., & Griggs, D. S. (1988). Age and visual search: Expanding the useful field of view. Journal of Optical Society of America A, 5(12), 2210-2219. DOI: 10.1364/josaa.5.002210 

  4. Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5(4), 323-370. DOI: 10.4135/9781412956253.n55 

  5. Belham, F. S., Satler, C., Garcia, A., Tomaz, C., Gasbarri, A., Rego, A., & Tavares, M. C. H. (2013). Age-related differences in cortical activity during a visuo-spatial working memory task with facial stimuli. PLoS One, 8(9), e75778. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0075778 

  6. Bieliauskas, L. A. (2001). General cognitive changes with aging. In Behavioral neurology in the elderly (pp. 85-108). CRC Press, Boca, Boca Raton. DOI: 10.1201/b14249-5 

  7. Calder, A. J., Young, A. W., Keane, J., & Dean, M. (2000). Configural information in facial expression perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(2), 527. DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.26.2.527 

  8. Calder, A. J., Keane, J., Manly, T., Sprengelmeyer, R., Scott, S., Nimmo-Smith, I., & Young, A. W. (2003). Facial expression recognition across the adult life span. Neuropsychologia, 41(2), 195-202. DOI: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00149-5 

  9. Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., & Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking time seriously: A theory of socio-emotional selectivity. American Psychologist, 54(3), 165. DOI: 10.1037/0003-066x.54.3.165 

  10. Carstensen, L. L., & Mikels, J. A. (2005). At the intersection of emotion and cognition: Aging and the positivity effect. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(3), 117-121. DOI: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00348.x 

  11. Clapp, W. C., Rubens, M. T., Sabharwal, J., & Gazzaley, A. (2011). Deficit in switching between functional brain networks underlies the impact of multitasking on working memory in older adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(17), 7212-7217. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1015297108 

  12. Coeckelbergh, T. R., Cornelissen, F. W., Brouwer, W. H., & Kooijman, A. C. (2004). Age-related changes in the functional visual field: Further evidence for an inverse age×eccentricity effect. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 59(1), 11-18. DOI: 10.1093/geronb/59.1.p11 

  13. Cohn, N. B., Dustman, R. E., & Bradford, D. C. (1984). Age-related decrements in stroop color test performance. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 40(5), 1244-1250. DOI: 10.1002/1097-4679(198409)40:5 3.0.co;2-d 

  14. Comalli Jr, P. E., Wapner, S., & Werner, H. (1962). Interference effects of Stroop color-word test in childhood, adulthood, and aging. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 100(1), 47-53. DOI: 10.1080/00221325.1962.10533572 

  15. Czigler, I., Csibra, G., & Ambro, A. (1996). Aging, stimulus identification and the effect of probability: an event-related potential study. Biological Psychology, 43(1), 27-40. DOI: 10.1016/0301-0511(95)05173-2 

  16. Donkers, F. C., & Van Boxtel, G. J. (2004). The N2 in go/no-go tasks reflects conflict monitoring not response inhibition. Brain and Cognition, 56(2), 165-176. DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.04.005 

  17. Duzel, E., Penny, W. D., & Burgess, N. (2010). Brain oscillations and memory. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 20(2), 143-149. DOI: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.01.004 

  18. Ekman, P. (1992). Are there basic emotions? Psychological Review, 99, 550-553. DOI: 10.1037/0033-295x.99.3.550 

  19. Estes, Z., & Adelman, J. S. (2008). Automatic vigilance for negative words is categorical and general. Emotion, 8, 453-457. DOI: 10.1037/a0012887 

  20. Falkenstein, M., Koshlykova, N. A., Kiroj, V. N., Hoormann, J., & Hohnsbein, J. (1995). Late ERP components in visual and auditory Go/Nogo tasks. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology/Evoked Potentials Section, 96(1), 36-43. DOI: 10.1016/0013-4694(94)00182-k 

  21. Falkenstein, M., Hoormann, J., & Hohnsbein, J. (2002). Inhibition-related ERP components: variation with modality, age, and time-on-task. Journal of Psychophysiology, 16(3), 167. DOI: 10.1027/0269-8803.16.3.167 

  22. Fallgatter, A. J., Mueller, T. J., & Strik, W. K. (1999). Age-related changes in the brain electrical correlates of response control. Clinical Neurophysiology, 110(5), 833-838. DOI: 10.1016/s1388-2457(99)00022-x 

  23. Gajewski, P. D., Stoerig, P., & Falkenstein, M. (2008). ERP-correlates of response selection in a response conflict paradigm. Brain Research, 1189, 127-134. DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.10.076 

  24. Garcia-Larrea, L., Lukaszewicz, A. C., & Mauguiere, F. (1992). Revisiting the oddball paradigm. Non-target vs neutral stimuli and the evaluation of ERP attentional effects. Neuropsychologia, 30(8), 723-741. DOI: 10.1016/0028-3932(92)90042-k 

  25. Gazzaley, A., & Nobre, A. C. (2012). Top-down modulation: bridging selective attention and working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(2), 129-135. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.014 

  26. Gilboa-Schechtman, E., Foa, E. B., & Amir, N. (1999). Attentional biases for facial expressions in social phobia: The face-in-the-crowd paradigm. Cognition & Emotion, 13(3), 305-318. DOI: 10.1080/026999399379294 

  27. Gola, M., Kaminski, J., Brzezicka, A., & Wrobel, A. (2012). Beta band oscillations as a correlate of alertness-changes in aging. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 85(1), 62-67. DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.09.001 

  28. Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2006). Effect of action video games on the spatial distribution of visuospatial attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(6), 1465. DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.32.6.1465 

  29. Greenwood, P. M., Parasuraman, R., & Alexander, G. E. (1997). Controlling the focus of spatial attention during visual search: Effects of advanced aging and Alzheimer disease. Neuropsychology, 11(1), 3. DOI: 10.1037/0894-4105.11.1.3 

  30. Greenwood, P. M., & Parasuraman, R. (1999). Scale of attentional focus in visual search. Perception & Psychophysics, 61(5), 837-859. DOI: 10.3758/bf03206901 

  31. Greenwood, P. M., & Parasuraman, R. (2004). The scaling of spatial attention in visual search and its modification in healthy aging. Perception & Psychophysics, 66(1), 3-22. DOI: 10.3758/bf03194857 

  32. Gruhn, D., Smith, J., & Baltes, P. B. (2005). No aging bias favoring memory for positive material: Evidence from a heterogeneity-homogeneity list paradigm using emotionally toned words. Psychology and Aging, 20(4), 579. DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.4.579 

  33. Hartley, A. A., Kieley, J. M., & Slabach, E. H. (1990). Age differences and similarities in the effects of cues and prompts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(3), 523. DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.16.3.523 

  34. Hammerer, D., Li, S. C., Muller, V., & Lindenberger, U. (2010). An electrophysiological study of response conflict processing across the lifespan: Assessing the roles of conflict monitoring, cue utilization, response anticipation, and response suppression. Neuropsychologia, 48(11), 3305-3316. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.07.014 

  35. Hillyard, S. A., & Anllo-Vento, L. (1998). Event-related brain potentials in the study of visual selective attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95(3), 781-787. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.95.3.781 

  36. Hultsch, D. F., Hertzog, C., Small, B. J., McDonaldMiszczak, L., & Dixon, R. A. (1992). Short-term longitudinal change in cognitive performance in later life. Psychology and Aging, 7(4), 571. DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.7.4.571 

  37. Hulur, G., Ram, N., Willis, S. L., Schaie, K. W., & Gerstorf, D. (2015). Cognitive dedifferentiation with increasing age and proximity of death: Within-person evidence from the Seattle Longitudinal Study. Psychology and Aging, 30(2), 311. DOI: 10.1037/a0039260 

  38. Hwang, M. J., & Jung, S. H. (2011). The ageing society of Korea and the population estimate. Korea Journal of Population Studies, 34(2), 113-133. 

  39. Isreal, J. B., Chesney, G. L., Wickens, C. D., & Donchin, E. (1980). P300 and tracking difficulty: Evidence for multiple resources in dual-task performance. Psychophysiology, 17(3), 259-273. DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1980.tb00146.x 

  40. Jodo, E., & Kayama, Y. (1992). Relation of a negative ERP component to response inhibition in a Go/No-go task. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 82(6), 477-482. DOI: 10.1016/0013-4694(92)90122-x 

  41. Joormann, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2006). Is this happiness I see? Biases in the identification of emotional facial expressions in depression and social phobia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115(4), 705. DOI: 10.1037/0021-843x.115.4.705 

  42. Kim, S. Y., Jung, J. B., & Nam, K. C. (2018). Variability in the Visuo-spatial Attention Ability and Emotional Perception Ability Related with Bipolar Disorder Tendency in the Normal Pupulation. Science of Emotion & Sensibility, 21(2), 145-158. DOI: 10.14695/kjsos.2018.21.2.145 

  43. Klein, M., Ponds, R. W., Houx, P. J., & Jolles, J. (1997). Effect of test duration on age-related differences in Stroop interference. Journal of Clinical and Experi-Mental Neuropsychology, 19(1), 77-82. DOI: 10.1080/01688639708403838 

  44. Kosslyn, S. M., Brown, H. D., & Dror, I. E. (1999). Aging and the scope of visual attention. Gerontology, 45(2), 102-109. DOI: 10.1159/000022071 

  45. LaBar, K. S., Gitelman, D. R., Parrish, T. B., & Mesulam, M. M. (1999). Neuroanatomic overlap of working memory and spatial attention networks: a functional MRI comparison within subjects. Neuroimage, 10(6), 695-704. DOI: 10.1006/nimg.1999.0503 

  46. Langley, L. K., Friesen, C. K., Saville, A. L., & Ciernia, A. T. (2011). Timing of reflexive visuospatial orienting in young, young-old, and old-old adults. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73(5), 1546-1561. DOI: 10.3758/s13414-011-0108-8 

  47. Lavie, N., & De Fockert, J. (2005). The role of working memory in attentional capture. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(4), 669-674. DOI: 10.3758/bf03196756 

  48. Li, S. C., & Lindenberger, U. (1999). Cross-level unification: A computational exploration of the link between deterioration of neurotransmitter systems and dedifferentiation of cognitive abilities in old age. In Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory (pp. 103-146). Hogrefe & Huber. 

  49. Li, S. C., Lindenberger, U., & Sikstrom, S. (2001). Aging cognition: from neuromodulation to representation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(11), 479-486. DOI: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01769-1 

  50. Lucci, G., Berchicci, M., Spinelli, D., Taddei, F., & Di Russo, F. (2013). The effects of aging on conflict detection. PloS one, 8(2), e56566. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0056566 

  51. MacPherson, S. E., Phillips, L. H., & Della Sala, S. (2002). Age, executive function and social decision making: A dorsolateral prefrontal theory of cognitive aging. Psychology and Aging, 17(4), 598. DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.4.598 

  52. Mangun, G. R., & Hillyard, S. A. (1990). Allocation of visual attention to spatial locations: Tradeoff functions for event-related brain potentials and detection performance. Perception & Psychophysics, 47(6), 532-550. DOI: 10.3758/bf03203106 

  53. Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Aging and attentional biases for emotional faces. Psychological Science, 14(5), 409-415. DOI: 10.1111/1467-9280.01455 

  54. McCalley, L. T., Bouwhuis, D. G., & Juola, J. F. (1995). Age changes in the distribution of visual attention. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 50(6), P316-P331. DOI: 10.1093/geronb/50B.6.P316 

  55. McDowell, C. L., Harrison, D. W., & Demaree, H. A. (1994). Is right hemisphere decline in the perception of emotion a function of aging?. International Journal of Neuroscience, 79(1-2), 1-11. DOI: 10.3109/00207459408986063 

  56. Mikels, J. A., Larkin, G. R., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., & Carstensen, L. L. (2005). Divergent trajectories in the aging mind: Changes in working memory for affective versus visual information with age. Psychology and Aging, 20(4), 542. DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.4.542 

  57. Mishra, J., & Gazzaley, A. (2013). Preserved discrimination performance and neural processing during crossmodal attention in aging. PLoS One, 8(11), e81894. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0081894 

  58. Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (1999). Orienting of attention to threatening facial expressions presented under conditions of restricted awareness. Cognition & Emotion, 13(6), 713-740. DOI: 10.1080/026999399379050 

  59. Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., & Damsma, E. (2000). Threat perception bias in nonreferred, socially anxious children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 29(3), 348-359. DOI: 10.1207/s15374424jccp2903_6 

  60. Naghavi, H. R., & Nyberg, L. (2005). Common fronto-parietal activity in attention, memory, and consciousness: Shared demands on integration?. Consciousness and Cognition, 14(2), 390-425. DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2004.10.003 

  61. Nee, D. E., & Jonides, J. (2009). Common and distinct neural correlates of perceptual and memorial selection. Neuroimage, 45(3), 963-975. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.005 

  62. Nobre, A. C., & Kastner, S. (2014). The Oxford handbook of attention. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199675111.001.0001 

  63. Novak, G., Ritter, W., & Vaughan Jr, H. G. (1992). Mismatch detection and the latency of temporal judgments. Psychophysiology, 29(4), 398-411. DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1992.tb01713.x 

  64. O'Connell, R. G., Balsters, J. H., Kilcullen, S. M., Campbell, W., Bokde, A. W., Lai, R., Upton, N., & Robertson, I. H. (2012). A simultaneous ERP/fMRI investigation of the P300 aging effect. Neurobiology of Aging, 33(10), 2448-2461. DOI: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.12.021 

  65. Onton, J., Delorme, A., & Makeig, S. (2005). Frontal midline EEG dynamics during working memory. Neuroimage, 27(2), 341-356. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.014 

  66. Palermo, R., & Rhodes, G. (2007). Are you always on my mind? A review of how face perception and attention interact. Neuropsychologia, 45(1), 75-92. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.025 

  67. Park, D. C., Lautenschlager, G., Hedden, T., Davidson, N. S., Smith, A. D., & Smith, P. K. (2002). Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychology and Aging, 17(2), 299. DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.2.299 

  68. Petrican, R., Moscovitch, M., & Schimmack, U. (2008). Cognitive resources, valence, and memory retrieval of emotional events in older adults. Psychology and Aging, 23(3), 585. DOI: 10.1037/a0013176 

  69. Phillips, L. H., MacLean, R. D., & Allen, R. (2002). Age and the understanding of emotions: Neuropsychological and sociocognitive perspectives. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 57(6), 526-530. DOI: 10.1093/geronb/57.6.p526 

  70. Picton, T. W., Stuss, D. T., Champagne, S. C., & Nelson, R. F. (1984). The effects of age on human event-related potentials. Psychophysiology, 21(3), 312-326. DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1984.tb02941.x 

  71. Potts, G. F. (2004). An ERP index of task relevance evaluation of visual stimuli. Brain and Cognition, 56(1), 5-13. DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.03.006 

  72. Pourtois, G., Grandjean, D., Sander, D., & Vuilleumier, P. (2004). Electrophysiological correlates of rapid spatial orienting towards fearful faces. Cerebral Cortex, 14(6), 619-633. DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhh023 

  73. Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., & Cappell, K. A. (2008). Neurocognitive aging and the compensation hypothesis. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(3), 177-182. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00570.x 

  74. Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., & Park, D. C. (2010). Human neuroscience and the aging mind: A new look at old problems. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 65(4), 405-415. DOI: 10.1093/geronb/gbq035 

  75. Roberts, L. E., Rau, H., Lutzenberger, W., & Birbaumer, N. (1994). Mapping P300 waves onto inhibition: Go/No Go discrimination. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology/Evoked Potentials Section, 92(1), 44-55. DOI: 10.1016/0168-5597(94)90006-x 

  76. Rozin, P., & Royzman, E. B. (2001). Negativity bias, negativity dominance, and contagion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5(4), 296-320. DOI: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0504_2 

  77. Ruffman, T., Henry, J. D., Livingstone, V., & Phillips, L. H. (2008). A meta-analytic review of emotion recognition and aging: Implications for neuropsychological models of aging. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 32(4), 863-881. DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.01.001 

  78. Santesso, D. L., Meuret, A. E., Hofmann, S. G., Mueller, E. M., Ratner, K. G., Roesch, E. B., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2008). Electrophysiological correlates of spatial orienting towards angry faces: A source localization study. Neuropsychologia, 46(5), 1338-1348. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.12.013 

  79. Satler, C., & Tomaz, C. (2011). Emotional working memory in Alzheimer's disease patients. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders Extra, 1(1), 124-138. DOI: 10.1155/2014/207698 

  80. Seiple, W., Szlyk, J. P., Yang, S., & Holopigian, K. (1996). Age-related functional field losses are not eccentricity dependent. Vision Research, 36(12), 1859-1866. DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(95)00288-x 

  81. Shimokawa, A., Yatomi, N., Anamizu, S., Torii, S., Isono, H., Sugai, Y., & Kohno, M. (2001). Influence of deteriorating ability of emotional comprehension on interpersonal behavior in Alzheimer-type dementia. Brain and Cognition, 47(3), 423-433. DOI: 10.1006/brcg.2001.1318 

  82. Spell, L. A., & Frank, E. (2000). Recognition of nonverbal communication of affect following traumatic brain injury. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24(4), 285-300. DOI: 10.1023/a:1006675230193 

  83. Staub, B., Doignon-Camus, N., Bacon, E., & Bonnefond, A. (2014). The effects of aging on sustained attention ability: An ERP study. Psychology and Aging, 29(3), 684. DOI: 10.1037/a0037067 

  84. Sullivan, S., & Ruffman, T. (2004). Emotion recognition deficits in the elderly. International Journal of Neuroscience, 114(3), 403-432. DOI: 10.1080/00207450490270901 

  85. Surcinelli, P., Codispoti, M., Montebarocci, O., Rossi, N., & Baldaro, B. (2006). Facial emotion recognition in trait anxiety. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 20(1), 110-117. DOI: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2004.11.010 

  86. Tales, A., Muir, J. L., Bayer, A., & Snowden, R. J. (2002). Spatial shifts in visual attention in normal ageing and dementia of the Alzheimer type. Neuropsychologia, 40(12), 2000-2012. DOI: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00057-x 

  87. Thomas, R. C., & Hasher, L. (2006). The influence of emotional valence on age differences in early processing and memory. Psychology and Aging, 21(4), 821. DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.4.821 

  88. Townsend, R. E., & Johnson, L. C. (1979). Relation of frequency-analyzed EEG to monitoring behavior. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 47(3), 272-279. DOI: 10.1016/0013-4694(79)90279-7 

  89. Vallesi, A., Stuss, D. T., McIntosh, A. R., & Picton, T. W. (2009). Age-related differences in processing irrelevant information: evidence from event-related potentials. Neuropsychologia, 47(2), 577-586. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.10.018 

  90. Vallesi, A., & Stuss, D. T. (2010). Excessive subthreshold motor preparation for non-target stimuli in normal aging. Neuroimage, 50(3), 1251-1257. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.022 

  91. Vallesi, A. (2011). Targets and non-targets in the aging brain: A go/nogo event-related potential study. Neuroscience Letters, 487(3), 313-317. DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.10.046 

  92. Vecera, S. P., & Rizzo, M. (2003). Spatial attention: Normal processes and their breakdown. Neurologic Clinics, 21(3), 575-607. DOI: 10.1016/s0733-8619(02)00103-2 

  93. Wascher, E., Falkenstein, M., & Wild-Wall, N. (2011). Age related strategic differences in processing irrelevant information. Neuroscience Letters, 487(1), 66-69. DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.09.075 

  94. Wild-Wall, N., Falkenstein, M., & Gajewski, P. D. (2012). Neural correlates of changes in a visual search task due to cognitive training in seniors. Neural Plasticity, 529057. DOI: 10.1155/2012/529057 

  95. Wong, B., Cronin-Golomb, A., & Neargarder, S. (2005). Patterns of visual scanning as predictors of emotion identification in normal aging. Neuropsychology, 19(6), 739. DOI: 10.1037/0894-4105.19.6.739 

  96. Zanto, T. P., Hennigan, K., Ostberg, M., Clapp, W. C., & Gazzaley, A. (2010). Predictive knowledge of stimulus relevance does not influence top-down suppression of irrelevant information in older adults. Cortex, 46(4), 564-574. DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.08.003 

저자의 다른 논문 :

관련 콘텐츠

이 논문과 함께 이용한 콘텐츠

저작권 관리 안내
섹션별 컨텐츠 바로가기

AI-Helper ※ AI-Helper는 오픈소스 모델을 사용합니다.

AI-Helper 아이콘
AI-Helper
안녕하세요, AI-Helper입니다. 좌측 "선택된 텍스트"에서 텍스트를 선택하여 요약, 번역, 용어설명을 실행하세요.
※ AI-Helper는 부적절한 답변을 할 수 있습니다.

선택된 텍스트

맨위로